[Date Prev][Date Next][Thread Prev][Thread Next][Date Index][Thread Index]

Colors, sounds, etc. (No OTR)



Rhys wrote:

> i am visually oriented in some ways and colour is a big part of my life. i
>  think sounds have color.
>  
>  ideas have shapes and colors.
>  
>  i am weird.

Nah, that's not weird.  It reminded me of this article from Time.com. (Pasted 
below.)
Getting way off topic here, but this was too cool not to share... I associate 
sounds with colors and shapes also, but not to this extent.  Wish I did... ;)

Anita
~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~

May 21, 2001
Ah, the Blue Smell of It! 

By Unmesh Kher

When Sean Day hears the wail of a saxophone, he sees a writhing mass of 
neon-purple snakes hovering in the air. The hum of a harmonica, on the other 
hand, has a pleasantly greenish hue, while plunking pianos evoke a fine blue 
mist. Eating is colorful too. When Day takes a spoonful of mango sorbet, the 
wall before him turns lime green, rippled with cherry-red stripes.

Day is no human kaleidoscope. Nor is the Taiwan-based linguist afflicted with 
anything you would call a disorder. He's a synesthete--one of a small group 
of otherwise ordinary citizens who perceive the world in extraordinary ways. 
Synesthesia is a kind of crossing of sensory signals in which the stimulation 
of one sense evokes another; purple may smell like kiwi; the aroma of mint 
may feel like glass; letters and digits might scroll by in Technicolor. This 
week a few dozen synesthetes and the psychologists who study them will gather 
at Princeton University for the first meeting of the American Synesthesia 
Association--a society devoted to furthering research into the phenomenon in 
the hopes that it will reveal something about the inner workings of the human 
mind.

Although the society is new, physicians and scholars have known about the 
condition for centuries. History, in fact, teems with brilliant 
synesthetes--including such luminaries as novelist Vladimir Nabokov, composer 
Franz Liszt and physicist Richard Feynman. Synesthesia enjoyed a certain 
spiritual currency in the late 19th century, especially among the European 
avant-garde. Many artists, most notably abstract painter Wassily Kandinsky, 
were famed for their synesthetic pretensions. "I saw all my colors," wrote 
Kandinsky, recalling his experience of a Wagner opera. "Wild lines verging on 
the insane formed drawings before my very eyes." 

Scientists have only recently begun to unravel the mysteries of synesthesia. 
They estimate that roughly 1 in 2,000 people has the condition and that there 
are nearly as many types of synesthesia as there are permutations of the 
senses. While synesthetic responses are usually as unique as fingerprints, 
the condition runs in families. Nabokov, for example, for whom the letter b 
evoked the color burnt sienna, and t, pistachio green, often argued with his 
equally synesthetic mother about the true colors of the alphabet.

Such revelations are not surprising. When synesthetes are asked to link 
letters and words to their corresponding hues, the responses tend to be 
precise. (The n Feynman read in his physics equations wasn't just violet; it 
was "mildly violet-bluish.") Tested a year later, synesthetes report the same 
colors 9 times out of 10; control groups repeat them just a third of the 
time. 

Brain imaging shows that this consistency has a physical basis. Words 
activate not only the language centers of the synesthete's brain--as they 
would in anybody else's--but also the vision- and color-processing centers. 
What remains open to debate, however, is how intimately the synesthetic 
response is tied to consciousness.

Recent studies of synesthetes who see colors in response to numbers and 
letters provide conflicting answers. Last year Mike Dixon of the University 
of Waterloo reported that merely imagining a number was enough to provoke 
synesthesia. And in March Australian researcher Jason Mattingley reported in 
Nature that the conscious recognition of a number is crucial to the 
generation of color. "You have to be aware of the meaning of what you see to 
experience synesthesia," says Dixon. 

Not so, say Vilayanur Ramachandran and Edward Hubbard of the University of 
California at San Diego. Their studies take advantage of a perceptual quirk: 
when an image in the periphery of our visual field is surrounded by similarly 
shaped and colored images, the brain has trouble registering its 
presence--even though the eye picks it up. They reported at a meeting of the 
Vision Sciences Society in Sarasota, Fla., last week that even when 
synesthetes can't "see" a peripheral image--say a 5 that's "crowded" by 
3s--they see the color associated with the digit in question. That suggests 
that synesthesia occurs in the earliest stages of perception--before the 
brain ascribes meaning to what the eye reports.

What does all this tell us about the mind? "What you're seeing here is a 
window into thought itself," says Ramachandran, who is slated to speak at the 
Princeton meeting. "It also gives us an experimental handle to investigate 
the neural basis of more elusive phenomena like metaphor." It's a fair bet, 
he argues, that synesthesia is caused by genetic mutations that create dense 
neural connections between areas of the brain that process sensory 
information. Ramachandran hypothesizes that in normal brains, a handful of 
these links might play a role in the formulation of metaphors, which often 
blend sensory elements of language (consider "sharp cheese" or "bitter 
cold"). That, he says, may explain why synesthesia is far more common among 
novelists, painters and poets than in the general population. And, perhaps, 
why the rest of us, who don't experience the world as synesthetes do, can 
still take pleasure in their visions. 

---------------
Unsubscribe by going to http://www.actwin.com/OtR/

Follow-Ups: